Download PDF
Mini-Review  |  Open Access  |  18 Sep 2025

Exploring cardiac physiological synchrony and its implications for stress and anxiety

Views: 65 |  Downloads: 3 |  Cited:  0
Ageing Neur Dis. 2025;5:20.
10.20517/and.2025.14 |  © The Author(s) 2025.
Author Information
Article Notes
Cite This Article

Abstract

When two individuals share a common experience, their heartbeats can become synchronized in a phenomenon known as cardiac physiological synchrony (CPS). This interpersonal coupling of heart rate and heart rate variability is considered a component of social behavior and may facilitate close emotional, physical, and prosocial interactions. However, the exact mechanisms underlying CPS are unknown, including its potential differential roles in various types of empathy. Notably, CPS is distinct from the direct electrical coupling that occurs between cardiomyocytes. We therefore review the current literature on CPS and the hypothesized mechanisms driving heartbeat synchronization. The consequences of either reduced or increased CPS during social interaction are not well studied. Existing evidence suggests that CPS may tune social interaction, potentially promoting prosociality or alleviating stress and anxiety depending on context. We hypothesize that variations in CPS may correspond to different types of empathy and could be linked to specific types of anxiety. Finally, we discuss the implications of CPS for mental health, therapeutic interventions, and strategies aimed at reducing chronic stress and anxiety. A deeper understanding of CPS mechanisms and consequences could help to noninvasively improve mental and physical well-being and mitigate structural degeneration in brain regions essential for memory and cognition.

Keywords

Physiological coupling, physiological synchrony, affective empathy, cognitive empathy, empathy, stress, anxiety

INTRODUCTION

Cardiac physiological synchrony (CPS) refers to the phenomenon in which individuals exhibit coordinated or coupled cardiac activity patterns, such as heart rate or heart rate variability, in social or shared contexts. This synchrony reflects underlying mechanisms that can be broadly categorized into three domains:

(1) Cognitive/attentional mechanisms: CPS emerges from shared attention, emotional engagement, or alignment of mental states during social interactions or collective experiences;

(2) Mechanical/vibrational entrainment: Passive physiological coupling occurs via physical signals such as vibrations, respiration, or other bodily rhythms that influence cardiac timing;

(3) Environmental modulation: External factors potentially influence autonomic nervous system dynamics and thereby modulate CPS.

Section 1 explores these categories in detail, highlighting their distinct mechanisms, conditions of operation, and implications for social behavior and health [Table 1].

Table 1

Circumstances and limitations under which different mechanisms hypothesized to contribute to CPS may occur [Cognitive-attentional: (a); Passive-physical entrainment: (b); Environmental modulation: (c)]

Mechanism Conditions under which it operates Known limitations References
(a) Attentional entrainment via audiovisual stimuli Requires attentional engagement with shared stimuli (e.g., narrative, music) Requires sustained attention; not driven by co-presence or respiration [14,15]
(a) Passive entrainment via shared experiences (non-interactive) Occurs when individuals independently process similar stimuli with aligned attention Weaker coupling if attention is low; mechanisms are unclear without interaction [15]
(a) Sensorimotor/movement-based coupling HR synchronization arises during synchronized voluntary or observed movement tasks (e.g., finger tapping) Requires precise timing/sensory feedback; persistence without cues remains unclear [12]
(b) Oscillatory-system entrainment (external periodic forcing, e.g., Vibration-based entrainment) Weak mechanical vibrations (e.g., during sleep or external application) can entrain heart rhythms. The heart acts like a relaxation oscillator; entrainment has been reported within ±3% of intrinsic HR Narrow effective frequency range reported so far; may not apply to irregular rhythms [7,22,28,29,41]
(c) Environmental modulation Solar or geomagnetic fluctuations may modulate HR and HRV, potentially affecting CPS dynamics Mechanisms remain speculative; reported effects are inconsistent [31,32]

‘Social behaviors’ refer to the ways individuals interact with each other, including communication, cooperation, competition, and conflict. These behaviors are shaped by both biological and psychological factors and are essential for building relationships, forming communities, and maintaining societal structures. Empathy is a key component of social behavior and can be divided into cognitive and affective dimensions[1]. Cognitive empathy involves understanding another person’s thoughts and emotions without necessarily experiencing them. Affective empathy involves experiencing emotions that mirror those of others, creating a deeper emotional connection. Both forms of empathy are influenced by learning from past situations and shared social contexts. During social interactions, individuals can also connect physiologically through the coupling of biological processes, including heart rate, heart rate variability, skin conductance, body temperature, and respiratory sinus arrhythmia (for a review, see[2]). Research indicates a strong link between high levels of affective empathy and increased general anxiety[3], while individuals with less accurate cognitive empathy tend to exhibit higher social anxiety[4]. This review highlights the mechanisms thought to induce CPS and the various contexts in which it occurs. Understanding how CPS is generated and its specific roles in social interactions may help leverage CPS as a noninvasive therapeutic tool to improve mental health.

Following the discussion of CPS mechanisms, Section 2 examines potential implications and applications, particularly regarding stress and anxiety. Given the established links among social connection, physiological coupling, and emotional well-being, CPS presents promising avenues for therapeutic intervention. This section explores the role of CPS in modulating stress and anxiety, its relevance in social and clinical contexts, and emerging interventions that leverage physiological synchrony to enhance psychological outcomes.

As demonstrated in the Graphical Abstract, multiple mechanisms contribute to CPS entrainment, including attentional states and environmental influences. Interpersonal synchrony can manifest through CPS, neural coherence (such as oscillatory activity and mirror neuron interactions), emotional mimicry, and endocrine responses like cortisol co-regulation. Such synchrony may, in turn, influence social behaviors, including empathy, stress, and anxiety, while creating feedback loops that affect both physiological and psychological states at intra- and interpersonal levels.

YOUR HEAD OR YOUR HEART? SUGGESTED MECHANISMS AND CONTEXTS OF CARDIAC PHYSIOLOGICAL SYNCHRONY

CPS refers to the unconscious alignment of heart rate (HR) and heart rate variability (HRV) among individuals during shared experiences. The underlying drivers of this synchrony remain unclear, and it is not yet known whether it reflects a direct cardiac function or arises indirectly through cognitive or respiratory processes. Evidence suggests that higher CPS is linked to improved teamwork[5,6] and stronger group cohesion[7]. Physical contact may enhance cardio-respiratory coupling, reducing pain[8], and it has been proposed that ‘Super Synchronizers’ (individuals with a heightened sensitivity to synchronizing physiological responses) are rated as more attractive to potential partners in speed-dating tasks[9]. CPS may also reflect affective empathy, arising from subconscious mirroring of internal states, facial expressions, and posture[10,11]. Clarifying the mechanisms of CPS may help us understand its benefits and therapeutic value in mitigating stress, loneliness, and anxiety, both in social contexts and in solitude [Table 1]. This review examines the evidence for the mechanisms proposed to induce CPS.

Cognitive/attentional mechanisms

Audiovisual stimuli

Shared experiences can produce synchronized neural processing among individuals, potentially resulting in correlated brain and heart rate patterns[12-14]. Several studies have shown that when individuals experience the same environmental stimuli - such as listening to a story[15], enjoying music[13], or tapping their fingers in rhythm[12] - their brains process information similarly, and their heart rates begin to correlate. For example, CPS during storytelling has been correlated with improved recall of the narrative, suggesting a link to attentional engagement or the degree of immersion in the experience[15].

Two studies employing finger-tapping or video-watching tasks showed that physiological coupling is modulated by attention and predicts memory performance, but is not driven by respiratory patterns[15,16]. One study specifically indicated that attentional engagement in a shared experience is necessary for CPS, with only signals correlated with brain activity - such as gaze position, pupil size, HR, and saccade rate - predicting physiological coupling, whereas respiration and head movements showed no correlation[16]. Further supporting the role of attention, CPS in response to audiovisual stimuli, particularly visual cues, can occur even without direct interaction or simultaneous exposure to the stimulus[11,15]. CPS has even been observed when participants watched the same emotional movie separately[17,18]. Interestingly, similar human-horse CPS appears to occur only when the horse attends to the human rather than its peripheral environment[19].

Movement

CPS may also serve nonsocial functions, related to sensorimotor synchronization within individuals for the rhythmic coordination of perception and action[9]. However, it can enhance social experiences by interacting with mirror neurons in the brain, which modulate activity both when an individual performs an action and when they observe a similar action[11,20]. Specifically, the cardiac cycle appears to couple with both voluntary motor actions and the observer’s perception of these actions. In the executor, movements occur out of phase with heartbeats, while in the observer, heartbeats occur out of phase with the executor’s movements[21]. This suggests that CPS may actively contribute to social interaction by potentially coordinating facial expressions, postures, or group movements[10,11].

Mechanical/vibrational entrainment

CPS can also occur in the absence of visual or cognitive engagement, suggesting that direct mechanical or vibrational signals may facilitate coupling between individuals. For example, CPS of HR and HRV has been observed between co-sleeping partners[22], with couples also exhibiting synchronization of rapid eye movement (REM) and non-REM sleep stages[23]. One possible mechanism is weak ballistocardiographic vibration transmitted through the bed surface. Similarly, fetal–maternal CPS has been attributed to entrainment by acoustic stimuli arising from maternal blood vessel pulsation, with stronger coupling occurring at higher maternal breathing rates that may fall within a frequency range optimal for the fetal cardiac system[24].

These findings are consistent with considering CPS within the framework of an oscillatory system, in which the heart can be synchronized by rhythmic external inputs. At the cellular level, donor cardiomyocytes have been shown to align their contraction rate with host cardiomyocytes via gap junction formation[25]. The heart has been likened to a relaxation oscillator, capable of entrainment by external forces such as vibration[22], acoustic signals[24], or low-power electrical stimulation[26,27], provided that the periodicity closely matches its intrinsic rhythm. Experimental evidence indicates that small deviations in stimulation frequency (approximately ±3%) are more effective in modulating heart rhythm than larger mismatches (±5%-10%)[28,29]. Additionally, interoceptive accuracy appears to influence autonomic responsiveness, with individuals more adept at perceiving their own HR showing stronger modulation in response to external stimuli[30].

Environment

Environmental impact

Environmental factors, including solar and geomagnetic activity, can also influence physiological mechanisms such as HRV. For instance, increased solar wind intensity has been linked to increased HR, potentially signaling a biological stress response. Although CPS is thought to be important for social interactions, environmental variability could add complexity to how coupling emerges[31-33].

CPS appears to be more directly associated with affective empathy, likely through shared autonomic regulation during emotional resonance. Stronger HR and HRV coupling have been observed in contexts involving emotional sharing (e.g., touch, shared pain, narrative immersion)[34]. By contrast, CPS may relate to cognitive empathy through processes such as joint attention, shared task engagement, or perspective-taking, which indirectly modulate physiological states [Table 1]. Future studies exploring the relationship between CPS and different forms of empathy, and whether they are mediated by distinct mechanisms, may provide deeper insight into the role of CPS and its applications in daily life in relation to stress and anxiety.

IMPLICATIONS OF CARDIAC PHYSIOLOGICAL SYNCHRONY ON STRESS AND ANXIETY

CPS and its reciprocal relationship with stress and anxiety make it a promising noninvasive therapeutic avenue with broad applications [Table 2]

Table 2

Applications of physiological coupling in therapy and stress and anxiety contexts

Application Description Status/Notes References
Couples and attachment anxiety Higher CPS observed in partners with elevated attachment anxiety; may buffer anxiety-related effects on immune and stress responses Hypothesized mechanism; requires further clinical validation [36,37]
Social identity and prejudice reduction Manipulating social identity can enhance physiological synchrony between strangers, potentially improving social bonds Early-stage experimental evidence; clinical impact untested [43]
SAD Lower CPS linked to SAD; CPS biofeedback proposed as a potential diagnostic or therapeutic tool Theoretical framework; clinical trials pending [39]
Therapist-patient synchrony during psychotherapy/ caregiver-recipient synchrony Increased skin conductance and heart rate coupling observed during sessions; correlates with self-reported alliance Correlational findings; causality and clinical impact not yet established [43,44]
Closed-loop vibration stimulation during sleep Imperceptible vibration at near-heart rate frequency to modulate autonomic activity and improve sleep quality or post-sleep arousal Preliminary pilot studies; further replication required [28,29]
HRV biofeedback Training to increase HRV via slow-paced breathing and real-time feedback; associated with reduced stress and anxiety Expanding evidence base [51]

CPS and mental health

Social isolation is known to increase loneliness, stress, and anxiety. It is therefore reasonable to presume that improving socialization may be beneficial to emotional and physical well-being[35]. Given the proposed role of CPS in social interaction[6,7,9,11], increasing CPS may be considered a therapeutic method to promote well-being by reducing stress and anxiety. Stronger CPS has been observed between partners with higher attachment anxiety, potentially serving as a mechanism to mitigate anxiety in specific social contexts, as attachment anxiety is also known to negatively affect immune responses and cortisol release[36,37]. Another study showed that manipulating perceptions of social identity can enhance physiological synchrony between relative strangers and even predict friendship interest. These findings suggest that prejudice-reduction interventions could aim to foster physiological synchrony[38]. Interestingly, in such cases, CPS appears to mediate a transition from negative to positive social interactions- reducing potential conflict- rather than merely strengthening relationships or attraction.

Further research is needed on ‘leading synchrony’, which refers to the degree to which each partner drives synchrony, and on how susceptible different people are to synchronization. Leading synchrony is dependent upon task type, sex, and social anxiety status[36,39]. In people without social anxiety disorder (SAD), higher levels of social anxiety are associated with increased CPS, which may guide appropriate social behavior. Conversely, SAD is linked to reduced CPS, which correlates with negative relational outcomes such as seeming emotionally distant or unresponsive[39,40]. Reduced CPS in SAD is thought to result either from self-focused attention, which prevents shared engagement with external stimuli, or from the suppression of emotional expression and physiological responses as a form of self-concealment. Both mechanisms could interfere with the ability to generate CPS with others. Consequently, measuring CPS has been proposed as a potential diagnostic tool, and biofeedback interventions aimed at increasing CPS have been suggested for SAD. As SAD is more prevalent in females[41] and positively correlates with social camouflaging in adults with autism[42], therapeutic induction of CPS may be particularly beneficial in certain populations.

CPS in therapeutic settings

Physiological synchronization has also been investigated between therapists and patients, with increased skin conductance coupling observed during cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT)[43]. Self-reported ratings have likewise been found to correlate with measured CPS during psychotherapy sessions[21]. While it remains unclear whether increased CPS during therapy directly improves patient outcomes, the therapist-patient alliance is known to play a crucial role in the therapeutic process[44]. Enhancing CPS may therefore improve self-reported experiences and strengthen the therapeutic relationship. Treating or preventing anxiety may help reduce the risk of dementia, or alleviate its earliest symptoms and delay progression[45,46]. In dementia, neuropsychiatric symptoms have been linked to the closeness of the caregiver-care recipient relationship, and CPS has been proposed as a mechanism that could foster this closeness and therefore reduce symptoms[47,48]. Furthermore, a meta-analysis of people with Alzheimer’s disease reported a decline in cognitive empathy, whereas emotional empathy remained unaffected[49].

CPS-related interventions

Because CPS during sleep is thought to occur through ballistocardiographic vibrations transmitted via the bed[22], one study tested closed-loop vibration stimulation at near-heart rate frequency to determine its potential benefits for sleep. The vibrations were delivered at an imperceptible intensity and set at +3% relative to HR, with the aim of activating the sympathetic nervous system to promote arousal. The study suggested that such stimulation may facilitate cognitive recovery after sleep, as indicated by faster Psychomotor Vigilance Task reaction times and higher self-reported arousal scores. In contrast, another study applied closed-loop vibration stimulation at -3% relative to HR, which successfully entrained participants’ HR to the external rhythm and increased parasympathetic activity. These effects were specific to the -3% condition and were not observed at -5% or -10%[28,29]. Together, these studies suggest that CPS-based interventions during sleep can be tailored either to enhance sleep quality via parasympathetic activation or to promote post-sleep arousal via sympathetic activation.

HRV biofeedback is also gaining popularity, as it allows individuals to monitor and regulate their HRV using real-time feedback. Slow-paced breathing (SPB) - reducing breathing rate to six breaths per min - is one approach shown to increase HRV[50]. HRV biofeedback training has been linked to substantial reductions in self-reported stress and anxiety[50,51]. Unlike interpersonal CPS, HRV biofeedback provides individuals with a target (the feedback signal) to synchronize with, thereby increasing HRV to a predetermined setpoint. This requires not only controlled breathing but also attentional focus on the breathing process while monitoring feedback as HRV changes. Such training can be delivered via mobile applications, enabling CPS-like effects even in the absence of social partners. Additionally, biofeedback may enhance interoceptive accuracy, which has been shown to influence heart rate responses to visual or auditory stimuli presented in rhythm with the cardiac cycle[30].

DISCUSSION

Interpersonal synchrony refers to the alignment of physiological, neural, and endocrine processes between individuals during social interaction. Although the molecular and cellular mechanisms remain unclear, numerous physiological changes are hypothesized to be sensitive to synchrony. Heart rate, respiration, and HRV can spontaneously synchronize, reflecting coupled autonomic activity that interacts with hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis reactivity and buffers stress responses[52,53]. Synchronized oscillatory activity in frontal, parietal, and temporal regions has been observed during joint attention, cooperation, and emotional sharing, with mirror neuron systems facilitating shared affective states that influence HPA activation[54,55]. Endocrine co-regulation is also evident in romantic partners and parent-infant dyads, where cortisol rhythms and acute stress responses align across individuals, indicating direct interpersonal HPA axis coupling[53,56]. Emotional contagion via microexpressions, vocal prosody, and posture further propagates shared autonomic nervous system (ANS) and HPA activation, linking emotional connection to coordinated physiological states[57].

The observed associations between CPS and higher therapy-patient ratings in clinical sessions[21,43], improved REM synchronization in co-sleepers[22,23], and increased friendship interest[43] underscore the potential of CPS as a mechanism to enhance mental and physical well-being. As CPS is a noninvasive phenomenon, interventions designed to promote synchrony could offer promising avenues for reducing loneliness, anxiety, and depression[35]. CPS could be particularly beneficial for older populations experiencing social isolation, as anxiety is associated with a 29% increased risk of dementia[35]. Anxiety and chronic stress cause structural degeneration and dysfunction of brain regions involved in spatial memory and cognition, particularly the hippocampus and prefrontal cortex (PFC). Such degeneration may contribute to the heightened risk of neuropsychiatric disorders, including depression and dementia[45]. Preventing or treating anxiety could therefore reduce dementia risk, alleviate early symptoms, and delay disease progression[46].

A key limitation in CPS research lies in establishing causality. Correlation-based measures of physiological synchrony, such as the Single Session Index (SSI), quantify concurrent covariation in biosignals and have been consistently linked to shared emotional engagement in dyads and groups[58,59]. However, these metrics are symmetric and do not provide directionality. By contrast, information-theoretic approaches such as Transfer Entropy (TE) and its symbolic variants (Symbolic Transfer Entropy, STE; Normalized STE, NSTE) estimate the extent to which the past of one signal improves prediction of another signal’s future beyond its own history, enabling inference of directed statistical dependencies[60-63]. TE/NSTE asymmetry can identify leader–follower relationships and moments of directed interpersonal influence in biosignals such as electrodermal activity (EDA, complementing SSI by providing directionality and sensitivity to temporal dynamics rather than absolute values. Nonetheless, TE/NSTE capture statistical information flow rather than definitive mechanistic causation and remain vulnerable to confounds such as shared external drivers or indirect pathways. Rigorous inference therefore requires control analyses (e.g., conditional/multivariate TE), surrogate testing, and ideally, experimental perturbations[63,64].

CONCLUSION

CPS arises during shared experiences and likely corresponds to affective empathy[10,11]. Although its exact mechanisms remain debated, CPS may depend on cognitive attention in scenarios such as listening to music[13], finger tapping[12], or human-horse interactions[19]. In contrast, CPS during sleep or fetal-mother interactions may rely on alternative factors, including brain state and weak noninvasive forces (e.g., mechanical vibrations)[22]. Further research is needed to illuminate the pathways underlying CPS, its role in social and emotional functioning, and its therapeutic potential. Distinguishing between cognitive ‘top-down’ mechanisms and sensory (e.g., mechanical) mechanisms of action is essential for developing CPS-based interventions, as these may play different roles in cognitive versus affective empathy. Moreover, elucidating the relationship between CPS, empathy, and other physiological markers such as oxytocin and cortisol helps determine whether CPS exerts causal or merely correlative effects on social behavior. Both cognitive and affective empathy have been positively correlated with higher levels of cardiac activity[21,65]. Thus, investigating the physiological and psychological mechanisms of CPS presents important opportunities for developing noninvasive therapeutic strategies to improve mental health.

DECLARATIONS

Acknowledgments

Hassaan-Ul Huda contributed to the initial discussion on the review topic.

Authors’ contributions

Definition of the review topic: Escobar LS, Jung SM

Collection of relevant research materials: Escobar LS, Jung SM

Editing: Escobar LS, Jung SM

The first version draft: Robbins M

Initial collection of research materials: Hwang J

Idea development: Hwang J

Manuscript arrangement and editing: Park DJ

Expertise contribution for manuscript improvement: Park DJ

Availability of data and materials

Not applicable.

Financial support and sponsorship

None.

Conflicts of interest

All authors are engaged in research as part of LYEONS, developing an intervention aimed at autonomic nervous system regulation. This study is conducted within the scope of the company’s research and development efforts. Escobar LS, Robbins M, Hwang J, and Jung SM are affiliated with LYEONS Ltd. Robbins M is a Reviewer of Aging and Neurodegenerative Diseases.

Ethical approval and consent to participate

Not applicable.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Copyright

© The Author(s) 2025.

REFERENCES

1. Deutsch F, Madle RA. Empathy: historic and current conceptualizations, measurement, and a cognitive theoretical perspective. Hum Dev. 1975;18:267-87.

2. Palumbo RV, Marraccini ME, Weyandt LL, et al. Interpersonal Autonomic Physiology: A Systematic Review of the Literature. Pers Soc Psychol Rev. 2017;21:99-141.

3. Gambin M, Sharp C. Relations between empathy and anxiety dimensions in inpatient adolescents. Anxiety Stress Coping. 2018;31:447-58.

4. Tibi-Elhanany Y, Shamay-Tsoory SG. Social cognition in social anxiety: first evidence for increased empathic abilities. Isr J Psychiatry Relat Sci. 2011;48:98-106.

5. Chanel G, Kivikangas JM, Ravaja N. Physiological compliance for social gaming analysis: Cooperative versus competitive play. Interac Comput. 2012;24:306-16.

6. Sharika KM, Thaikkandi S, Nivedita , Platt ML. Interpersonal heart rate synchrony predicts effective information processing in a naturalistic group decision-making task. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2024;121:e2313801121.

7. Gordon I, Gilboa A, Cohen S, et al. Physiological and behavioral synchrony predict group cohesion and performance. Sci Rep. 2020;10:8484.

8. Goldstein P, Weissman-Fogel I, Shamay-Tsoory SG. The role of touch in regulating inter-partner physiological coupling during empathy for pain. Sci Rep. 2017;7:3252.

9. Cohen M, Abargil M, Ahissar M, Atzil S. Social and nonsocial synchrony are interrelated and romantically attractive. Commun Psychol. 2024;2:57.

10. Oliver LD, Vieira JB, Neufeld RWJ, Dziobek I, Mitchell DGV. Greater involvement of action simulation mechanisms in emotional vs cognitive empathy. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2018;13:367-80.

11. Jospe K, Genzer S, Klein Selle N, Ong D, Zaki J, Perry A. The contribution of linguistic and visual cues to physiological synchrony and empathic accuracy. Cortex. 2020;132:296-308.

12. Flory S, Guglielmini S, Scholkmann F, Marcar VL, Wolf M. How our hearts beat together: a study on physiological synchronization based on a self-paced joint motor task. Sci Rep. 2023;13:11987.

13. Nomura R. Reliability for music-induced heart rate synchronization. Sci Rep. 2024;14:12200.

14. Bason PT, Celler BG. Control of the heart rate by external stimuli. Nature. 1972;238:279-80.

15. Pérez P, Madsen J, Banellis L, et al. Conscious processing of narrative stimuli synchronizes heart rate between individuals. Cell Rep. 2021;36:109692.

16. Madsen J, Parra LC. Cognitive processing of a common stimulus synchronizes brains, hearts, and eyes. PNAS Nexus. 2022;1:pgac020.

17. Golland Y, Arzouan Y, Levit-Binnun N. The mere co-presence: synchronization of autonomic signals and emotional responses across co-present individuals not engaged in direct interaction. PLoS One. 2015;10:e0125804.

18. Golland Y, Keissar K, Levit-Binnun N. Studying the dynamics of autonomic activity during emotional experience. Psychophysiology. 2014;51:1101-11.

19. Callara AL, Scopa C, Contalbrigo L, et al. Unveiling directional physiological coupling in human-horse interactions. iScience. 2024;27:110857.

20. Rizzolatti G, Fadiga L, Gallese V, Fogassi L. Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res. 1996;3:131-41.

21. Palser ER, Glass J, Fotopoulou A, Kilner JM. Relationship between cardiac cycle and the timing of actions during action execution and observation. Cognition. 2021;217:104907.

22. Yoon H, Choi SH, Kim SK, et al. Human heart rhythms synchronize while Co-sleeping. Front Physiol. 2019;10:190.

23. Drews HJ, Wallot S, Brysch P, et al. Bed-sharing in couples is associated with increased and stabilized rem sleep and sleep-stage synchronization. Front Psychiatry. 2020;11:583.

24. Van Leeuwen P, Geue D, Thiel M, et al. Influence of paced maternal breathing on fetal-maternal heart rate coordination. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2009;106:13661-6.

25. Rubart M, Pasumarthi KB, Nakajima H, Soonpaa MH, Nakajima HO, Field LJ. Physiological coupling of donor and host cardiomyocytes after cellular transplantation. Circ Res. 2003;92:1217-24.

26. der Tweel LH, Meijler FL, van Capelle FJ. Synchronization of the heart. J Appl Physiol. 1973;34:283-7.

27. Anishchenko VS, Balanov AG, Janson NB, Igosheva NB, Bordyugov GV. Entrainment between heart rate and weak noninvasive forcing. Int J Bifurcation Chaos. 2000;10:2339-48.

28. Choi SH, Yoon H, Jin HW, et al. Effect of closed-loop vibration stimulation on heart rhythm during naps. Sensors (Basel). 2019;19:4136.

29. Son HW, Kim SH, Lee TM, et al. Closed-loop vibration stimulation based on heart rhythm for reducing sleep inertia. J Sleep Res. 2025;:e14458.

30. Tanaka Y, Terasawa Y, Umeda S. Effects of interoceptive accuracy in autonomic responses to external stimuli based on cardiac rhythm. PLoS One. 2021;16:e0256914.

31. Alabdulgader A, McCraty R, Atkinson M, et al. Long-term study of heart rate variability responses to changes in the solar and geomagnetic environment. Sci Rep. 2018;8:2663.

32. Ramishvili A, Janashia K, Tvildiani L. High heart rate variability causes better adaptation to the impact of geomagnetic storms. Atmosphere. 2023;14:1707.

33. McCraty R, Atkinson M, Stolc V, Alabdulgader AA, Vainoras A, Ragulskis M. Synchronization of human autonomic nervous system rhythms with geomagnetic activity in human subjects. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2017;14:770.

34. Bastos MAV Jr, Braz DF, Porto ALM, Cordeiro KSDS, Portella RB, Granger DA. Physiological attunement and flourishing: understanding the influence of relationships on health. Front Psychiatry. 2025;16:1614379.

35. Wolters NE, Mobach L, Wuthrich VM, et al. Emotional and social loneliness and their unique links with social isolation, depression and anxiety. J Affect Disord. 2023;329:207-17.

36. Helm JL, Sbarra D, Ferrer E. Assessing cross-partner associations in physiological responses via coupled oscillator models. Emotion. 2012;12:748-62.

37. Pietromonaco PR, DeBuse CJ, Powers SI. Does attachment get under the skin? Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2013;22:63-8.

38. Danyluck C, Page-gould E. Intergroup dissimilarity predicts physiological synchrony and affiliation in intergroup interaction. J Exp Soc Psychol. 2018;74:111-20.

39. Asher M, Barthel AL, Hofmann SG, Okon-Singer H, Aderka IM. When two hearts beat as one: heart-rate synchrony in social anxiety disorder. Behav Res Ther. 2021;141:103859.

40. Konrad K, Gerloff C, Kohl SH, et al. Interpersonal neural synchrony and mental disorders: unlocking potential pathways for clinical interventions. Front Neurosci. 2024;18:1286130.

41. Asher M, Asnaani A, Aderka IM. Gender differences in social anxiety disorder: a review. Clin Psychol Rev. 2017;56:1-12.

42. Hull L, Levy L, Lai MC, et al. Is social camouflaging associated with anxiety and depression in autistic adults? Mol Autism. 2021;12:13.

43. Gernert CC, Nelson A, Falkai P, Falter-Wagner CM. Synchrony in psychotherapy: High physiological positive concordance predicts symptom reduction and negative concordance predicts symptom aggravation. Int J Methods Psychiatr Res. 2023;33:e1978.

44. Tschacher W, Ribeiro E, Gonçalves A, Sampaio A, Moreira P, Coutinho J. Electrodermal synchrony of patient and therapist as a predictor of alliance and outcome in psychotherapy. Front Psychol. 2025;16:1545719.

45. Mah L, Szabuniewicz C, Fiocco AJ. Can anxiety damage the brain? Curr Opin Psychiatry. 2016;29:56-63.

46. Santabárbara J, Lipnicki DM, Villagrasa B, Lobo E, Lopez-Anton R. Anxiety and risk of dementia: Systematic review and meta-analysis of prospective cohort studies. Maturitas. 2019;119:14-20.

47. Vernon EK, Cooley B, Rozum W, et al. Caregiver-care recipient relationship closeness is associated with neuropsychiatric symptoms in dementia. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2019;27:349-59.

48. Fu D, Incio-Serra N, Motta-Ochoa R, Blain-Moraes S. Interpersonal physiological synchrony for detecting moments of connection in persons with dementia: a pilot study. Front Psychol. 2021;12:749710.

49. Demichelis OP, Coundouris SP, Grainger SA, Henry JD. Empathy and theory of mind in Alzheimer’s Disease: a meta-analysis. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2020;26:963-77.

50. Goessl VC, Curtiss JE, Hofmann SG. The effect of heart rate variability biofeedback training on stress and anxiety: a meta-analysis. Psychol Med. 2017;47:2578-86.

51. Zucker TL, Samuelson KW, Muench F, Greenberg MA, Gevirtz RN. The effects of respiratory sinus arrhythmia biofeedback on heart rate variability and posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms: a pilot study. Appl Psychophysiol Biofeedback. 2009;34:135-43.

52. Butler EA, Randall AK. Emotional coregulation in close relationships. Emot Rev. 2013;5:202-10.

53. Feldman R. Parent-infant synchrony: a biobehavioral model of mutual influences in the formation of affiliative bonds. Monographs Society Res Child. 2012;77:42-51.

54. Dumas G, Nadel J, Soussignan R, Martinerie J, Garnero L. Inter-brain synchronization during social interaction. PLoS One. 2010;5:e12166.

55. Czeszumski A, Eustergerling S, Lang A, et al. Hyperscanning: a valid method to study neural inter-brain underpinnings of social interaction. Front Hum Neurosci. 2020;14:39.

56. Bastos Jr. MAV, Braz DF, Porto ALM, Cordeiro KSdS, Portella RB, Granger DA. Physiological attunement and flourishing: understanding the influence of relationships on health. Front Psychiatry. 2025;16:1614379.

57. Hatfield E, Cacioppo JT, Rapson RL. Emotional contagion. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 1993.

58. Marci CD, Orr SP. The effect of emotional distance on psychophysiologic concordance and perceived empathy between patient and interviewer. Appl Psychophysiol Biofeedback. 2006;31:115-28.

59. Slovák P, Tennent P, Fitzpatrick G. Exploring skin conductance synchronisation in everyday interactions. In NordiCHI '14: Proceedings of the 8th Nordic Conference on Human-Computer Interaction: Fun, Fast, Foundational; 2014 Oct 26-30; Helsinki, Finland. New York: Association for Computing Machinery; 2014. p. 511-20.

60. Schreiber T. Measuring information transfer. Phys Rev Lett. 2000;85:461-4.

61. Lee U, Oh G, Kim S, Noh G, Choi B, Mashour GA. Brain networks maintain a scale-free organization across consciousness, anesthesia, and recovery: evidence for adaptive reconfiguration. Anesthesiology. 2010;113:1081-91.

62. Faes L, Nollo G, Jurysta F, Marinazzo D. Information dynamics of brain-heart physiological networks during sleep. New J Phys. 2014;16:105005.

63. Takamizawa K, Kawasaki M. Transfer entropy for synchronized behavior estimation of interpersonal relationships in human communication: identifying leaders or followers. Sci Rep. 2019;9:10960.

64. Thorson KR, West TV, Mendes WB. Measuring physiological influence in dyads: A guide to designing, implementing, and analyzing dyadic physiological studies. Psychol Methods. 2018;23:595-616.

65. Slovák P, Tennent P, Fitzpatrick G. Physiological indicators of the relation between autistic traits and empathy: evidence from electrocardiogram and skin conductance signals. In ICBME 2022: Proceeding of 2022 29th National and 7th International Iranian Conference on Biomedical Engineering (ICBME); 2022 Dec 21-22; Tehran, Iran. Piscataway: IEEE Publishers; 2022. p. 177-83.

Cite This Article

Mini-Review
Open Access
Exploring cardiac physiological synchrony and its implications for stress and anxiety

How to Cite

Download Citation

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click on download.

Export Citation File:

Type of Import

Tips on Downloading Citation

This feature enables you to download the bibliographic information (also called citation data, header data, or metadata) for the articles on our site.

Citation Manager File Format

Use the radio buttons to choose how to format the bibliographic data you're harvesting. Several citation manager formats are available, including EndNote and BibTex.

Type of Import

If you have citation management software installed on your computer your Web browser should be able to import metadata directly into your reference database.

Direct Import: When the Direct Import option is selected (the default state), a dialogue box will give you the option to Save or Open the downloaded citation data. Choosing Open will either launch your citation manager or give you a choice of applications with which to use the metadata. The Save option saves the file locally for later use.

Indirect Import: When the Indirect Import option is selected, the metadata is displayed and may be copied and pasted as needed.

About This Article

© The Author(s) 2025. Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, for any purpose, even commercially, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made.

Data & Comments

Data

Views
65
Downloads
3
Citations
0
Comments
0
1

Comments

Comments must be written in English. Spam, offensive content, impersonation, and private information will not be permitted. If any comment is reported and identified as inappropriate content by OAE staff, the comment will be removed without notice. If you have any queries or need any help, please contact us at [email protected].

0
Download PDF
Share This Article
Scan the QR code for reading!
See Updates
Contents
Figures
Related
Ageing and Neurodegenerative Diseases
ISSN 2769-5301 (Online)

Portico

All published articles will be preserved here permanently:

https://www.portico.org/publishers/oae/

Portico

All published articles will be preserved here permanently:

https://www.portico.org/publishers/oae/